Two Voices on the Credibility Crisis in Psychology
Arkadiusz Białek
Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychologyhttps://orcid.org/0000-0002-9002-4764
Piotr Wolski
Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychologyhttps://orcid.org/0000-0002-7028-6142
Abstract
While various shortcomings and flaws in the conduct of research and analysis of results in psychology and other social sciences have been recognized for a long time, recent years have witnessed greater prevalence and wider scope of this criticism. There are also more proposals for improvement. In this article, we focus on selected, key areas of the credibility crisis in psychology. Piotr Wolski discusses those related to the improper understanding and application of significance tests, while Arkadiusz Białek characterizes some of the research practices that undermine the credibility of psychological studies and demonstrates how to counteract them. Although the use of good research practices can improve the reproducibility and replicability of research results, the proposed reform should also encompass the way theories are developed. The discussed proposal for theory development in psychology leads to a series of practical steps. Unlike the hypothetico-deductive model, it starts with the identification and description of the phenomenon. The explanation of the phenomenon formulated through abduction is then formalized in mathematical equations or computer simulations and verified. Adhering to good research practices and proper theory development has the potential to provide psychology with more solid foundations and make it a cumulatively evolving science.
Keywords:
credibility crisis, statistical inference, p-value, significance tests, questionable research practices, theory developmentReferences
Andrade, C. (2021). HARKing, Cherry-Picking, P-Hacking, Fishing Expeditions, and Data Dredging and Mining as Questionable Research Practices. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 82(1), 20f13804. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.20f13804
Crossref
Google Scholar
Bakker, M., van Dijk, A., & Wicherts, J. M. (2012). The Rules of the Game Called Psychological Science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 543–554.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Bakker, M., Hartgerink, C. H., Wicherts, J. M., & van der Maas, H. L. (2016). Researchers’ Intuitions About Power in Psychological Research. Psychological Science, 27(8), 1069–1077. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616647519
Crossref
Google Scholar
Beer, R. D. (2020). Lost in words. Adaptive Behavior, 28(1), 19–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712319867907
Crossref
Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (2019). Rein in the four horsemen of irreproducibility. Nature, 568(7753), 435. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-019-01307-2
Crossref
Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (2021). UBL & Elsevier seminars on Reproducible Research. YouTube https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=C-rk22as870&t=214s Google Scholar
Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L. J., Dalege, J., Kievit, R. A., & Haig, B. D. (2021). Theory Construction Methodology: A Practical Framework for Building Theories in Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 756–766. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620969647
Crossref
Google Scholar
Boyce, V., Mathur, M. B., & Frank, M. C. (2023, July 31). Eleven years of student replication projects provide evidence on the correlates of replicability in psychology. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dpyn6
Crossref
Google Scholar
Bringmann, L. F., Elmer, T., & Eronen, M. I. (2022). Back to Basics: The Importance of Conceptual Clarification in Psychological Science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(4), 340–346. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214221096485
Crossref
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1962). The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 65, 145–153.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1990). Things I have learned (so far). American Psychologist, 45, 1304–1312. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.12.1304
Crossref
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1994). The Earth is round (p < .05). American Psychologist, 49, 997–1003. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.12.997
Crossref
Google Scholar
Cumming, G. (2008). Replication and p Intervals: p Values Predict the Future Only Vaguely, but Confidence Intervals Do Much Better. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(4), 286–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00079.x
Crossref
Google Scholar
Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychological Science, 25(1), 7–29.
Crossref
Google Scholar
de Groot, A. D. (1956/2014). The meaning of “significance” for different types of research (translation and annotated by E.-J. Wagenmakers, D. Borsboom, J. Verhagen, R. Kievit, M. Bakker, A. Cramer, D. Matzke, D. Mellenbergh, & H. L. J. van der Maas), Acta Psychologica, 148, 188–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.001
Crossref
Google Scholar
Eronen, M. I., & Bringmann, L. F. (2021). The Theory Crisis in Psychology: How to Move Forward. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 779–788. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620970586
Crossref
Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03193146
Crossref
Google Scholar
Fidler, F., Singleton Thorn, F., Barnett, A., Kambouris, S., & Kruger, A. (2018). The Epistemic Importance of Establishing the Absence of an Effect. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2), 237–244. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918770407
Crossref
Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1971). The design of experiments (9th edition). Hafner Press. Google Scholar
Flake, J., & Fried, E. (2020). Measurement Schmeasurement: Questionable Measurement Practices and How to Avoid Them. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(4), 456–465. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245920952393
Crossref
Google Scholar
Fried, E., Flake, J., & Robinaugh, D. (2022). Revisiting the theoretical and methodological foundations of depression measurement. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 358–368. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00050-2
Crossref
Google Scholar
Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2014). The statistical crisis in science. American Scientist, 102, 460–465. https://doi.org/10.1511/2014.111.460
Crossref
Google Scholar
Haig, B. D. (2005). An Abductive Theory of Scientific Method. Psychological Methods, 10(4), 371–388. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.371
Crossref
Google Scholar
Haller, H., & Krauss, S. (2002). Misinterpretations of Significance: A Problem Students Share with Their Teachers? Methods of Psychological Research Online, 7(1). Google Scholar
Halsey, L. G., Curran-Everett, D., Vowler, S. L., & Drummond, G. B. (2015). The fickle P value generates irreproducible results. Nature Methods, 12(3), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3288
Crossref
Google Scholar
Haslbeck, J. M. B., Ryan, O., Robinaugh, D. J., Waldorp, L. J., & Borsboom, D. (2022). Modeling psychopathology: From data models to formal theories. Psychological Methods, 27(6), 930–957. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000303
Crossref
Google Scholar
Heyard, R. (2022). Best practices in statistical design and reporting. University of Zurich. https://osf.io/t9rqm/ Google Scholar
Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLOS Medicine, 2(8), e124. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
Crossref
Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L. (1998). HARKing: hypothesizing after the results are known. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(3), 196–217. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0203_4
Crossref
Google Scholar
Lakens, D. (2023, July 24). Concerns about Replicability, Theorizing, Applicability, Generalizability, and Methodology across Two Crises in Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dtvs7
Crossref
Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(4), 806–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.46.4.806
Crossref
Google Scholar
Munafò, M. R., Nosek, B. A., Bishop, D. V. M., Button, K. S., Chambers, C. D., Percie du Sert, N., Simonsohn, U., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ware, J. J., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). A manifesto for reproducible science. Nature Human Behaviour, 1.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Ebersole, C. R., DeHaven, A. C., & Mellor, D. T. (2018). The preregistration revolution. PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America), 115(11), 2600–2606. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1708274114
Crossref
Google Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Hardwicke, T. E., Moshontz, H., Allard, A., Corker, K. S., Dreber, A., Fidler, F., Hilgard, J., Struhl, M. K., Nuijten, M. B., Rohrer, J. M., Romero, F., Scheel, A. M., Scherer, L. D., Schönbrodt, F. D., & Vazire, S. (2022). Replicability, robustness, and reproducibility in psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 73, 719–748. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-114157
Crossref
Google Scholar
Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2019). Addressing the theory crisis in psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1596–1618. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01645-2
Crossref
Google Scholar
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), 1–8.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Robinaugh, D. J., Haslbeck, J. M. B., Ryan, O., Fried, E. I., & Waldorp, L. J. (2021). Invisible Hands and Fine Calipers: A Call to Use Formal Theory as a Toolkit for Theory Construction. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 725–743. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620974697
Crossref
Google Scholar
Rossi, J. S. (1990). Statistical power of psychological research: What have we gained in 20 years. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58(5), 646.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (2001). Social Psychology and Science: Some Lessons from Solomon Asch. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(1), 2–14. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_1
Crossref
Google Scholar
Scheel, A. (2022). Why most psychological research findings are not even wrong. Infant and Child Development, 31(1): e2295. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2295
Crossref
Google Scholar
Scheel, A. M., Tiokhin, L., Isager, P. M., & Lakens, D. (2021). Why hypothesis testers should spend less time testing hypotheses. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 744–755. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966795
Crossref
Google Scholar
Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1989). Do studies of statistical power have an effect on the power of studies? Psychological Bulletin, 105(2), 309.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E., Calanchini, J., & Pickett, C. L. (2015). Theory development with agent-based models. Organizational Psychology Review, 5(4), 300–317. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386614546944
Crossref
Google Scholar
Szollosi, A., & Donkin, C. (2021). Arrested Theory Development: The Misguided Distinction Between Exploratory and Confirmatory Research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 717–724. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966796
Crossref
Google Scholar
van Dongen, N. N. N., van Bork, R., Finnemann, A., van der Maas, H., Robinaugh, D., Haslbeck, J. M. B., … Borsboom, D. (2022, April 13). Productive Explanation: A Framework for Evaluating Explanations in Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qd69g
Crossref
Google Scholar
van Rooij, I., Baggio, G. (2020). Theory development requires an epistemological sea change. Psychological Inquiry, 31(4), 321–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2020.1853477
Crossref
Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J. Selker, R., Gronau, Q. F., Šmíra, M., Epskamp, S., Matzke, D., Rouder, J. N., & Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 35–57.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L. J., & Kievit, R. A. (2012). An Agenda for Purely Confirmatory Research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 632–638. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612463078
Crossref
Google Scholar
Wasserstein, R. (2015). ASA comment on a journal’s ban on null hypothesis statistical testing. Retrieved 05 Aug 2015, Sente. Google Scholar
Westover, M. B., Westover, K. D., & Bianchi, M. T. (2011). Significance testing as perverse probabilistic reasoning. BMC Medicine, 9, 20. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-9-20
Crossref
Google Scholar
Wilkinson, L., APA Task Force on Statistical Inference. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54(8), 594–604.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Wolski, P. (2016). Istotność statystyczna II. Pułapki interpretacyjne [Statistical significance II. Interpretive pitfalls]. Rocznik Kognitywistyczny [Yearbook of Cognitive Science], 9, 59–70 (in Polish). https://doi.org/10.4467/20843895RK.16.006.6412
Crossref
Google Scholar
Woolston, C. (2015). Psychology journal bans P values. Nature, 519(7541), 9.
Crossref
Google Scholar
Yarkoni, T. (2022). The generalizability crisis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, E1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20001685
Crossref
Google Scholar
Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9002-4764
License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.